Boys don鈥檛 cry, at least as far as they recall
When former U.S. Rep. Patricia Schroeder of Colorado tearfully announced in 1987 that she would not seek the Democratic nomination for president, many analysts leaped in to suggest that such an outward display of emotion made her unqualified for the office, anyway.
But what if all our tightly held stereotypes about 鈥渆motional鈥 females and stoic males are simply wrong? What if we鈥檙e all about the same when it comes to feeling and expressing emotions, but have allowed deeply ingrained stereotypes to sculpt how we remember what we feel?
That鈥檚 one striking conclusion in a new paper by Leaf Van Boven, associate professor in the University of Colorado Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, and his colleague Michael D. Robinson, M.D., of the Department of Psychology at North Dakota State University.
鈥淚t鈥檚 very widely believed that men and women have very different emotional experiences,鈥 Van Boven says. 鈥淏ut one of the puzzles that psychologists have noticed is that when you confront or show people a very distinct, emotionally arousing film, vignette, (or) novel, males and females don鈥檛 really differ in their emotional response.鈥
Where they do differ is in how they remember their feelings, the researchers conclude in 鈥淏oys don鈥檛 cry: Cognitive load and priming increase stereotypic sex difference in emotion memory,鈥 published in the January edition of the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.
Cueing off of previous research concluding that humans are more likely to resort to stereotypical judgments鈥攁bout, say, race or sex鈥攚hen they are distracted or otherwise under a cognitive load, Van Boven and Robinson designed studies to determine whether the same might be true of men鈥檚 and women鈥檚 recollection of their own emotional states.
In one study, CU undergraduates were shown an emotionally wrenching scene from one of two films: the moment in 鈥淪ophie鈥檚 Choice鈥 when a mother fleeing Nazi Germany (Meryl Streep) must choose between her two young children, or a scene in 鈥淲it,鈥 in which a woman dying of cancer (Emma Thompson) lies in a hospital bed while her mentor reads 鈥淭he Runaway Bunny鈥 to her.
鈥淚f you ask people to report their emotions right in the moment while watching (the scenes), there is no sex difference,鈥 Van Boven says. And when given 20 minutes or so to process the scenes without taxing themselves cognitively, both men and women reported feeling the feelings of sadness you might expect.
However, when test subjects were asked to memorize long strings of letters, requiring significant concentration before describing their emotional reactions, everything changed.
鈥淭he females recalled having experienced more intense emotions during the film,鈥 Van Boven says. 鈥淭he males reported less-intense feelings.鈥
In other words, when their brains were under stress, both men and women resorted to common stereotypes about how people of different gender experience emotion.
Interestingly, the researchers found that the pattern holds for anger, one of the few emotions people believe men experience more strongly than women. Although both sexes experienced the same intensity of anger in the moment, men recalled feeling more intense anger when asked to describe their emotions under cognitive stress鈥攂oth reverted to emotional stereotyping, in other words.
The work suggests that when it comes to how we experience emotions, we may be victims of our own, self-perpetuating stereotypes. And that can have reverberations throughout society, Van Boven says.
鈥淚n our culture, being emotional is not considered a very good thing,鈥 he says. 鈥淏ut the idea that men and women really differ (emotionally) is really exaggerated. 鈥 The evidence suggests that the exaggeration contributes to the perpetuation of stereotypes and possibly discrimination.鈥